Depression is a commonly misunderstood term. Specially the word is used in common lingo to describe anything from weakness to loneliness to sadness. But following concepts will let you know, what is NOT MDD!
With something so prevalent, many of us may assume we know what it is.
But assumptions can quickly turn to misconceptions. Misconceptions about what depression looks and feels like. Misconceptions about whether people really want to get better. Misconceptions about the seriousness of MDD. Which is important because depressive disorder is serious. It affects a person’s entire being. It affects their entire life.
Depression is NOT weakness:
In other words, it isn’t some inherent flaw or sign that someone is weak-minded, faint-hearted, too sensitive or a powerless wimp. Depression is an illness.
It is NOT a Choice:
we make the mistake of thinking people do. We know that we have the power to change our perspective and outlook. We can challenge and restructure our thoughts. We can make meaningful changes. But individuals with MDD can’t think away their sadness any more than someone with diabetes can lower their blood sugar with their mind. Both are illnesses that require intervention.
MDD is NOT just sadness:
MDD is a constellation of symptoms.
“Depression is such a cruel punishment. There are no fevers, no rashes, no blood tests to send people scurrying in concern, just the slow erosion of the self, as insidious as cancer. And like cancer, it is essentially a solitary experience: a room in hell with only your name on the door.”
MDD is NOT just to appear depressed.
Many people are very good at putting on a positive face in the morning and getting through their day, only to collapse into their MDD in the evening when they are home,” she said. People hold down jobs while struggling with suicidal thoughts, she said. We don’t know what resides behind someone’s exterior, no matter how put together it might be. We can’t read minds or see into hearts.
A maximum number of patients show great improvement. mild to moderate cases require a good counseling /therapy work. Medical treatment works on all degrees of MDD .
Alloway, R. & Bebbington, P. (1987) The buffer theory of social support – a review of the literature. Psychological Medicine, 17, 91–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1980) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn) (DSM–III). Washington, DC: APA.Google ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, R. C. & Jolley, D. J. (1986) The prognosis of depression in old age. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 574–583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blazer, D. G., Hughes, D. & George, L. K. (1987) Stressful life events and the onset of a generalized anxiety syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1178–1183.Google ScholarPubMed
Carney, M. W. P., Reynolds, E. H., & Sheffield, B. F. (1987) Prediction of outcome in depressive illness by the Newcastle Diagnosis Scale: its relationship with the unipolar/bipolar and DSM–III systems. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 43–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S. & Wills, T. A. (1985) Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975) “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 12, 189–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, A. S. (1984) Interpreting the evidence on social support. Social Psychiatry, 19, 49–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, A. S. & Moran, P. A. P. (1983) Social relationships during the onset and remission of neurotic symptoms: a prospective community study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 467–472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holahan, C. J. & Moos, R. H. (1981) Social support and psychological distress: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 365–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, D. C., Blazer, D. G., & George, L. K. (1988a) Age differences in life events: a multivariate controlled analysis. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 27, 207–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, D. C., George, L. K., & Blazer, D. G. (1988b) Age differences in life event qualities: multivariate controlled analyses. Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 161–174.3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. B. & Shapiro, R. W. (1981) Major depressive disorder: initial results from a one-year prospective naturalistic follow-up study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169, 761–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. B.Shapiro, R. W., Lavori, P. W., et al (1982) Recovery in major depressive disorder: analysis with the life table and regression models. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 905–910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landerman, R., George, L. K., Campbell, R. T., et al (1989) Social support, stress, and depression: alternative models of the stress buffering hypothesis. American Journal of Community Psychology(in press).Google Scholar
Murphy, E. (1983) The prognosis of depression in old age. British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 111–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pattison, E. M., Lleamas, R., & Hurd, G. (1979) Social network mediation of anxiety. Psychiatric Annals, 9, 56–67.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977) The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Crouchan, J., et al (1981) National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 381–389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, M., Coryell, W., & Pfohl, B. (1987) Prognostic validity of the dexamethasone suppression test: results of a six-month prospective follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 212–214.Google ScholarPubMed